TL;DR

  • David Reich’s genomic research suggests no single “brain mutation” sparked modern consciousness – a view mirrored by the Eve Theory of Consciousness (EToC), which attributes our self-awareness to cultural innovation rather than one genetic leap 1.
  • EToC posits a memetic origin of introspection: early humans “discovered” the concept of the self (the “I”) through altered-state rituals (notably using snake venom) and spread it culturally, with women likely pioneering this cognitive revolution 2 3.
  • This theory addresses the Sapient Paradox: the puzzling gap between anatomically modern humans (~200,000 years ago) and the much later explosion of art, religion, and complex culture. EToC offers a solution by suggesting a late prehistoric “Great Awakening” of consciousness that ignited symbolic culture worldwide 4 5.
  • If Reich read EToC today, he might be intrigued by its interdisciplinary evidence (myths, archaeology, genetics) and its testable predictions. He would likely appreciate that EToC’s gene–culture coevolution model aligns with recent ancient DNA findings showing selection on cognitive traits in the last 10,000 years 6, even as he remains cautious and calls for more empirical validation.

Ancient DNA and the Puzzle of Consciousness Emergence#

Modern humans looked anatomically human long before they acted fully human. Geneticists and anthropologists have long grappled with why behaviors like symbolic art, religion, and advanced language bloomed tens of millennia after our species first appeared. This disconnect – dubbed the Sapient Paradox – asks why anatomically modern Homo sapiens (present in Africa by ~300,000–200,000 years ago) only became behaviorally modern much later 7 8. In other words, what sparked the “light switch” that turned Homo sapiens into sapient beings capable of culture and consciousness as we know them?

The Great Leap Forward (or Lack Thereof)#

For decades, one theory held that a sudden genetic mutation triggered a “Great Leap Forward” in cognition about 50–100 thousand years ago. Prominent voices like paleoanthropologist Richard Klein and linguist Noam Chomsky have speculated that a single genetic change (perhaps aiding complex language or recursion) might have occurred in Africa and spread globally, catalyzing modern human behavior 9 10. Chomsky, for example, suggested that recursive grammar – the ability to embed thoughts within thoughts, a cornerstone of language – arose from a chance mutation in one individual, after which “the trek from Africa started” with fully modern minds 11 10. This would neatly explain why sophisticated art and tools proliferated worldwide after ~50k years ago.

David Reich, however, approaches this hypothesis with skepticism. As a leading population geneticist who has sequenced genomes from hundreds of ancient humans, Reich has looked for any sign of a ubiquitous late Pleistocene genetic “switch” – and mostly come up empty. In his 2018 book Who We Are and How We Got Here, Reich notes that apart from mitochondrial DNA and the Y-chromosome (which trace single lineages), there is no region in the nuclear genome where all humans share a common ancestor within the last ~100,000 years 12. If a single favorable mutation (for, say, recursive thought or grammar) had swept through our species in that timeframe, we would expect to see evidence of a recent common ancestor for the DNA around that gene. “But the key change, if it exists, is running out of places to hide,” Reich writes, alluding to the exhaustive genomic surveys that have turned up no obvious culprit 1. In plainer terms, our genomes don’t show signs of a singular “mental spark” mutation in the late Ice Age.

Instead, Reich is open to the idea that many mutations over time – perhaps guided by new cultural pressures – contributed to our cognitive advance 13. Complex traits like language ability or intelligence are highly polygenic (influenced by hundreds or thousands of genes), a fact underscored by modern studies of traits like schizophrenia and linguistics 14. Any evolutionary change in consciousness likely involved a gradual accumulation of small genetic tweaks rather than a miraculous single event. This stance aligns with archaeologists Sally McBrearty and Alison Brooks’ critique of Klein’s idea: key behaviors such as art and symbolism have roots well before 50k years ago, suggesting no overnight revolution but a piecemeal buildup 15.

Reich’s Timeline: Gradualism with a Mystery#

From a genetic perspective, Reich acknowledges something momentous did happen as Homo sapiens spread out of Africa. The archaeological record shows a dramatic acceleration of innovation after ~50,000 years ago: modern humans displaced Neanderthals and other archaic humans across Eurasia 16, and new artifacts like bone tools, figurative art, and personal ornaments burst forth 17. The simplest explanation, as Reich recounts, is that a culturally and cognitively advanced population expanded from Africa or the Near East, bringing a sophisticated new mindset that outcompeted indigenous hominins 18. In essence, a behavioral revolution piggybacked on a demographic expansion. But what drove that behavioral shift? If not a single gene, then what? This remains an open question in Reich’s narrative – a question that the Eve Theory of Consciousness boldly tries to answer from another angle.

Notably, Reich emphasizes that genetic transmission doesn’t require all traits to be ancient. Gene flow connects us more than one might guess: mathematically, the most recent common ancestor of all humans could have lived as recently as a few thousand years ago 19. This startling fact (often illustrated by the thought experiment that if a man from 10,000 years ago has any living descendants today, he could be ancestor to everyone 20) implies that even a trait arising in one region in the Mesolithic or Neolithic could, in theory, spread to all humanity via interbreeding and population movements. Reich would be aware of such models of rapid ancestry crossover 20. Thus, he wouldn’t rule out a relatively recent origin for a universal human trait if there was a mechanism for its spread – be it cultural diffusion, genetic selection, or both.

In summary, from Reich’s vantage point: human cognitive modernity likely emerged through a tapestry of small genetic changes and cultural developments, not a single mutation. He’s looking for what could have sparked the tipping point in our ancestors’ minds. Enter the Eve Theory of Consciousness, which offers a provocative hypothesis: that the spark wasn’t in our DNA at all – at least not initially – but in a cultural discovery so profound that it transformed our species.


The Eve Theory of Consciousness (EToC) – A Cultural “Genesis” of the Mind#

Eve Theory of Consciousness (EToC) is a far-reaching hypothesis by cognitive researcher Andrew Cutler that reframes the birth of human self-awareness as a historical event – one encoded in myth and enacted through ritual, rather than a slow biological evolution alone. The theory’s name invokes the Biblical Eve for symbolic reasons: just as Eve’s bite of the forbidden fruit in Genesis awakened Adam and Eve to knowledge (and to shame at their nakedness – a classic sign of self-awareness), EToC suggests that real humans “bit” into self-consciousness at a certain point in prehistory, irreversibly changing the human psyche. In effect, it proposes that there was a first generation to experience true introspective consciousness, and they passed this revelation to others. Where conventional science asks when did humans evolve consciousness, EToC asks instead: when did humans discover consciousness?

Myth and Memory: Clues of a Consciousness Revolution#

Cutler points to striking commonalities in ancient myths and religious symbols across the world as potential cultural memories of humanity’s awakening. For instance, many creation myths begin with an act of self-reference or naming: “In the beginning was the Word…” or “In the beginning, I…” 21. The Garden of Eden story in the Judeo-Christian tradition famously depicts the first man and woman attaining self-awareness (realizing their nakedness and facing exile from paradise) only after disobeying God and heeding a serpent. EToC takes these myths seriously—not literally as magic fruit or talking snakes, but as psychological fossils. The ubiquitous serpent motif, in particular, is no coincidence in Cutler’s view. He proposes that a snake-associated ritual lies at the heart of humanity’s transition to sapience 3 4.

The theory identifies snake venom as the primordial tool used to induce altered states and spark introspection. In a scenario that gives Terence McKenna’s “stoned ape” hypothesis a venomous twist, EToC suggests early humans discovered that the neurotoxic bite of a snake (perhaps taken in small, controlled doses or during shamanic ordeals) could trigger intense, mind-altering experiences – even out-of-body visions and a dissociation of self from body 22 3. In those harrowing trance states (bordering on life and death), a few pioneering individuals likely had the first flicker of reflective self-awareness: the realization “I am separate from my experience”. By mythic analogy, the snake “offered” knowledge of good and evil – in reality, knowledge of the self – and Eve (symbolizing the first conscious humans) partook.

Crucially, EToC argues that women were the initial discoverers of the inner self. Cutler’s thesis posits that “women discovered ‘I’ first and then taught men about inner life” 2. This conjecture stems from several angles: women’s unique roles in early societies (as gatherers, healers, or central figures in rituals like initiation and fertility rites), their evolutionary edge in social cognition and empathy, and even archaeological hints that link females to early symbolic artifacts. For example, many of the oldest hand stencils on cave walls – a proxy for whoever was creating Paleolithic art – were made by women (determined by finger length ratios) 23. EToC builds on this by suggesting female sages or shamans were the first to “taste” self-awareness (much as Eve was first to taste the fruit), and seeing its value, initiated men through mind-rending rites of passage 24 25. In other words, the knowledge of the self began as an esoteric, probably secret revelation – a “mystery cult” of consciousness.

Once this fire was lit, however, it spread “like wildfire” across human groups 26. Those who underwent the ritual emerged with a fundamentally changed cognition: an inner voice, a capacity for abstract thought, and awareness of mortality. The theory paints a dramatic picture of the immediate fallout. The birth of the self was a double-edged sword: it gave us planning, imagination, and empathy, but also death anxiety, existential angst, and mental illness previously unknown to creatures without inner selves 27 28. Early conscious humans, as EToC tells it, suddenly grappled with fears and desires of a new order – they could envision their own death, yearn for meaning, and scheme for future gains (leading to innovations like burials, art, personal property, and eventually agriculture 29 30). In the Eden story, this loss of innocence meant expulsion from unity with nature; in EToC’s narrative, it meant humans could no longer live “happily unconscious” like other animals. The human condition – with all its wonders and woes – had begun.

Memes First, Genes Later: A Consciousness “Contagion”#

One of the most compelling (and controversial) claims of EToC is that consciousness initially spread memetically, not genetically. In scientific terms, it was a case of cultural evolution driving biological evolution – a concept known as gene–culture coevolution. The idea is that the practice of self-awareness rituals (the “meme” or cultural trait) created a new selective pressure on our gene pool, favoring those individuals whose brains could best accommodate and stabilize this bizarre new trait of introspective ego.

At first, having an “inner voice” may have been a fragile, overwhelming, even maladaptive novelty for Homo sapiens. (Julian Jaynes, who famously theorized about a late emergence of consciousness, imagined that the first transition from a mind of automatic, external voices to a mind with an internal self could feel like insanity31.) EToC acknowledges this transitional chaos – pointing to archaeological oddities like the Neolithic epidemic of trepanned skulls (holes drilled in skulls) possibly meant to release “demons” from troubled minds 32. But eventually, what began as a cultural innovation – a taught mindset of selfhood – would kickstart natural selection for brains better adapted to this mode of thought 33 28. As Cutler puts it, once the concept of self (a “recursive” mode of thinking) took hold, “non-recursive or semi-recursive people could have evolved into the memetic niche in subsequent thousands of years” 34. In other words, any populations or individuals slow to acquire the new mind would be at a disadvantage relative to those “possessed” of the self-aware culture.

Over many generations, genes that supported things like recursive internal dialogue, theory of mind, longer attention spans, and emotional regulation in this new introspective context would be favored 35 36. EToC thus predicts a sort of Snowball effect: the cultural spark of “I am” spreads, and then genetic evolution accelerates it and locks it in. Cutler even suggests that archaic styles of cognition (sometimes dubbed “bicameral” minds, which lacked a singular introspective self) went extinct much like woolly mammoths did – unable to compete with the survival advantages conferred by introspective planning and collaboration 33. By the time of recorded history, the ancient teaching had become instinct: today, every child “reinherits” a self in early development, both by gene and enculturation.

From an evidentiary standpoint, what makes EToC unusual among theories of consciousness origins is that it is explicitly historical and interdisciplinary. It stakes out a position that can be tested and potentially falsified by various fields – archaeology, mythology, linguistics, neuroscience, and yes, genetics 37 38. The theory asserts, for example, that our myths of a golden age and a fall from innocence aren’t pure fiction but distant echoes of real psychological events 39. It argues many cultures share flood myths, serpent symbols, or “First Man and Woman” stories because those were the actual events and actors of the Great Awakening, diffused through migrating tribes 40 41. It further predicts we should find archaeological traces of early ritual centers or “snake cult” sites as crucibles of consciousness (Cutler highlights one candidate: Tsodilo Hills in Botswana, where a 70,000-year-old rock shaped like a python seems to have been a focus of ritual activity – perhaps one of humanity’s oldest snake ceremonies 42 43). On the genetic side, EToC ventures that the late Pleistocene and early Holocene should show signs of selection on brain-related genes – for instance, allele frequency shifts related to neural development, cognitive ability, or susceptibility to mental illness as a byproduct of our newly complex minds 5. These are bold claims, but they offer concrete avenues for scientists to investigate.

How would a data-driven geneticist like David Reich react to all this? Likely with a mix of fascination and healthy skepticism. EToC is a sweeping synthesis, drawing connections from ancient cave paintings to modern psychiatric disorders. For Reich, who deals in hard genomic data, the grand narrative alone wouldn’t be enough – he would zero in on which parts of this story can be supported (or refuted) by evidence. Fortunately, EToC provides several hook points for genetics and archaeology to latch onto. And intriguingly, some of the latest findings from Reich’s own field actually resonate with EToC’s timeline and mechanism.


Where Reich and EToC Converge: Memes, Genes, and Testable Clues#

If David Reich were to read the Eve Theory of Consciousness today, a number of its ideas would likely strike him as compelling or at least worthy of further inquiry. Here are several key points where Reich’s empirical outlook and Cutler’s hypothesis meaningfully intersect:

  1. No Single “Brain Gene” – but Many Small Ones: Both Reich and EToC reject the notion of a lone genetic mutation gifting humanity with modern cognition. Reich’s research found that no near-universal genetic change in the last 100k years can account for a sudden cognitive revolution 12 1. EToC echoes this by attributing the change to culture first and positing any genetic role to be gradual and polygenic. In fact, Reich’s comment that a key mutation is “running out of places to hide” in our DNA 44 neatly supports EToC’s core premise: that the catalyst wasn’t a gene at all, but a meme (the idea of “I”). Any genetic adaptation came later, via coordinated natural selection on many genes – a scenario Reich himself entertains as plausible 1.

  2. Gene–Culture Coevolution in Action: Reich is very familiar with gene–culture dynamics (e.g. how dairy farming led to selection for milk digestion genes). He might find EToC’s memes-first, genes-second model plausible, given evidence that culture can drive genetic change. Remarkably, a recent ancient DNA study co-authored by Reich examined over 8,000 genomes across 10,000 years and found signs that alleles linked to cognitive performance were increasingly favored in post-Ice Age Europe 45 46. For example, early European farmers had fewer genetic markers for schizophrenia (a mental illness tied to higher baseline creativity and dopamine) compared to their hunter-gatherer predecessors – suggesting natural selection was reducing certain cognitive side-effects as society became more complex 6. They also found that polygenic scores for educational attainment (correlated with intelligence) rose over time in these populations 45. These findings align strikingly with EToC’s narrative: after humans developed advanced culture (farming, towns, social stratification), there was selection against maladaptive extremes of our new consciousness (like psychosis) and perhaps for enhanced intellect. Reich would recognize this as supportive data that the Holocene epoch saw ongoing evolutionary tuning of our brains – exactly what EToC predicts as the aftermath of a late bloom of sapience 47.

  3. A Solution to the Sapient Paradox: Reich is acquainted with the puzzle of why cultural modernity appears long after anatomical modernity 17 9. EToC offers a concrete resolution: our ancestors had the brain hardware, but it required a cultural “software update” (the invention of self-reflective practices) to unlock its full potential. This would mean that features like art, symbolic language, and religion could indeed have a more recent origin without requiring a sudden brain mutation – they arose when the cognitive mode of our species shifted. Reich might find this compelling because it meshes with what the archaeological record actually shows: a patchwork, global timing of behavioral modernity. Some regions (like Europe and Indonesia) see an explosion of figurative art ~40,000 years ago, while others lagged, and certain innovations (agriculture, writing) only appear much later 48 15. If consciousness truly “switched on” at different times in different places via cultural transmission, it would explain these geographic and temporal disparities better than a mutation that should have impacted everyone at once. It also reframes the “Upper Paleolithic Revolution” not as a global overnight miracle, but as the spread of a revolutionary idea – one that took time to diffuse.

  4. Interdisciplinary Evidence and Falsifiability: A scientist like Reich would appreciate that EToC sticks its neck out by making bold predictions that other researchers can investigate. The theory doesn’t just rely on metaphor; it expects to be validated by hard evidence from multiple domains. For instance, EToC predicts that if we had a way to measure it, we’d see a spike in neurological and psychological stress markers as humans underwent this transition (perhaps explaining widespread trepanation in Neolithic skulls as a desperate remedy for newfound mental disturbances 49). It predicts that wherever the consciousness cult spread, we should find concurrent shifts in material culture – maybe sudden appearances of new burial practices, goddess or serpent figurines, or secretive initiation sites. It even ventures into genetics by predicting detectable shifts in allele frequencies for brain function in the late Pleistocene/Holocene 5. Reich, whose career is built on extracting historical stories from DNA, would likely commend this willingness to engage with genetics. The testability is key: as he knows, a hypothesis that bridges mythology, archaeology, and genetics can be wrong in many ways – but if it’s right, it will light up each of those radar screens with signal. EToC already lines up with some signals (e.g. selection on cognition by 10k years ago, global snake/dragon myths pointing to a common source). Reich might say: “The pieces are intriguing – let’s gather more data and see if the story holds up.”

  5. “Humanity’s Mother” and the Matriarchal Spark: Though outside Reich’s typical focus, the idea that women drove the initial spread of sapience might resonate with findings in anthropology and even subtle hints in genetics. Women, being primary caregivers and early social organizers, could have been natural teachers of the inner voice (for instance, a mother’s voice guiding a child may have been the template for the original “voice of god” in one’s head 50 51). Additionally, mitochondrial DNA – famously tracing a “Mitochondrial Eve” in Africa ~160,000 years ago 52 – reminds us that unbroken maternal lineages carry deep history. While that is a separate concept, Reich might muse on the poetry that one kind of Eve (genetic) gave us our bodies, and an allegorical Eve gave us our minds. At minimum, he would be intrigued by data like the cave-hand analyses showing female participation in creating the earliest art 23. EToC’s emphasis on a female-led knowledge transfer might prompt Reich to consider whether any genetic evidence (perhaps loci related to brain development on the X chromosome, or sex-differentiated selection pressures) correlates with this hypothesis. It’s a speculative aspect, but one grounded in the idea that who in society innovates can leave subtle traces (cultural or genetic) over time.

Of course, Reich would also have pointed questions and critiques. He might ask: if consciousness arose in one region relatively recently, how do we account for Australia’s Aboriginal Dreamtime myths or the rich spiritual art of Upper Paleolithic Europe without positing multiple independent “discoveries”? EToC would answer that the consciousness cult likely spread globally via migration and diffusion, or even arose in parallel once the initial spark set a trend – an answer needing evidence. He would emphasize the need to pin down when and where this proto-cult operated: Was it 70,000 years ago in Africa (as the Tsodilo Hills clue suggests) or much later, around the end of the Ice Age (~12,000 years ago) as some of Cutler’s writings imply 25? The difference is huge in genetic terms, and Reich would know that by 12k years ago, humans in the Americas and Oceania were isolated from the Old World. EToC might respond that the awakening could have begun earlier (e.g. 50–40k years ago, during the great human expansions) but only reached a critical mass at the dawn of the Neolithic – something for archaeologists to clarify.

In essence, Reich’s reaction would likely be that of a scientist intrigued by a bold hypothesis that could dovetail with what we know, but who insists on separating which parts are speculative from those grounded in solid data. His overall stance might be cautiously optimistic: the Eve Theory of Consciousness, while unconventional, aligns with the emerging view that our species’ defining cognitive traits evolved via a complex interplay of culture and genetics – not a single lucky mutation. It provokes exactly the kind of cross-disciplinary research questions that someone like Reich appreciates. After all, Reich has argued that we must be open to substantial biological differences and changes in human populations over time 53; EToC suggests one of those differences was when and how different groups became fully self-aware, and that’s a difference he could investigate with the tools of ancient DNA.

By engaging with EToC, Reich would find himself at the intersection of genomics and the humanities – reading not only genomes and fossils but also folklore and ritual for traces of our past. He might not accept every claim at face value (e.g. the literal snake venom mechanism might raise an eyebrow until more evidence emerges of ancient venom use), yet he would surely applaud the theory’s ambition. It attempts what few scientific theories do: to connect our genetic history with the story of our “soul.” For a researcher who has helped rewrite the story of our biological ancestry, the Eve Theory of Consciousness offers a provocative narrative about our psychological ancestry – one that he would regard with scientific curiosity and an open mind.


FAQ#

Q1. What evidence would David Reich look for to test the Eve Theory of Consciousness? A: He would likely seek genetic signals and archaeological data from the timeframe of the proposed “awakening.” For example, Reich might analyze ancient DNA for signs of selection on brain-related genes in the Late Pleistocene/Holocene 47, and look for correlations with archaeological indicators of symbolism or ritual (cave art, figurines, sacred sites) to see if they coincide with a genetic shift.

Q2. Why does EToC emphasize snake venom and could genetics support that idea? A: EToC hypothesizes that snake venom was an early psychedelic catalyst for self-awareness, symbolized by serpents in myth 3. While genetics can’t directly prove ritual snake bites, it might offer indirect support – for instance, if a gene variant for toxin resistance or relevant neurotransmitter pathways rose in frequency under selection. Reich would probably note this is speculative but testable if such genetic adaptations to neurotoxins are found in our ancestry.

Q3. Did David Reich ever propose a specific timeline for when human consciousness arose? A: Not explicitly – Reich has focused on when populations split and mixed, and he acknowledges the “behavioral modernity” puzzle without pinning it to a single date 9. He cites evidence of a cultural flourishing around 50,000 years ago but remains cautious about assigning it to one sudden cause. Reich leans toward a gradual accumulation of cognitive changes, leaving open the possibility of a drawn-out or multi-step emergence of full self-awareness.

Q4. How would Reich view the claim that “women discovered ‘I’ and taught it to men”? A: He’d find it interesting but would ask what evidence underpins it. While genetics doesn’t directly record which sex led an innovation, Reich might point out supportive clues like the high proportion of female handprints in Ice Age cave art 23 or studies showing women’s slight cognitive advantages in social perception. He would treat it as a hypothesis to explore with anthropological data (e.g. patterns of matriarchal myths or gender roles in ritual) rather than a proven fact.

Q5. Does the Eve Theory conflict with the Out-of-Africa model that Reich champions? A: Not fundamentally. The Out-of-Africa model (which Reich’s work helped confirm) describes the dispersal of humans from Africa ~50-60k years ago 54 16. EToC could complement this by suggesting that those dispersing humans did not instantly possess our full modern consciousness until a cultural breakthrough that might have occurred during or after the dispersal. Reich would see no conflict as long as the theory acknowledges Africa’s central role in human origins – it simply adds that a key cultural evolution (consciousness) may have blossomed subsequently and spread across those already dispersed populations via contact and selective advantage.


Footnotes#


Sources#

  1. Cutler, Andrew. “Eve Theory of Consciousness v3.0: How Humans Evolved a Soul.” Vectors of Mind, Feb 27, 2024. (Comprehensive essay outlining the Eve Theory, including its mythological, anthropological, and genetic evidence.)
  2. Cutler, Andrew. “The Ritualised Mind and the Eve Theory of Consciousness: A Convergent Account of Human Cognitive Evolution.” How Humans Evolved (snakecult.net), April 19, 2025. (Academic-style synthesis comparing Tom Froese’s ritual-origins model with EToC, and discussing testable predictions.)
  3. Reich, David. Who We Are and How We Got Here: Ancient DNA and the New Science of the Human Past. New York: Pantheon, 2018. (Reich’s book includes his perspectives on the spread of modern humans from Africa and the search for genetic explanations for the burst of modern behavior after ~50k years ago.)
  4. Klein, Richard. “Archaeology and the Evolution of Human Behavior.” Evolutionary Anthropology 9, no. 1 (2000): 17–36. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(2000)9:1<17::AID-EVAN3>3.0.CO;2-A (Background on the “behavioral modernity” debate, with Klein’s hypothesis of a genetic trigger ~50k years ago and counterarguments by other anthropologists.)
  5. Vyshedskiy, Andrey. “Language Evolution: How Language Revolutionized Cognition.” Psychology Research 7, no. 12 (2017): 791–814. PDF (An example of the theory that a sudden genetic change (perhaps 70k–50k years ago) facilitated recursive language and abstract thought, representing the “single mutation” viewpoint in the consciousness emergence debate.)
  6. Wynn, Thomas, and Frederick L. Coolidge. “The Rise of Homo Sapiens: The Evolution of Modern Thinking.” Wiley-Blackwell, 2009. (A scholarly overview of theories on how and when modern cognitive abilities evolved, including gradualist models and the role of cultural artifacts in detecting mind development.)
  7. Ramand, Phillip. “Creation Myths, Stoned Apes & the Eve Theory of Consciousness.” Seeds of Science, March 3, 2023. (Article discussing EToC in the context of other unconventional theories of consciousness origins, useful for understanding how EToC builds on or diverges from ideas like McKenna’s “stoned ape.”)
  8. Emil Kirkegaard, “Overwhelming evidence of recent evolution in West Eurasians,” Aporia Magazine, Sept 24, 2024. (Summary of Reich et al.’s 2024 ancient DNA study that found selection in the last 10k years on polygenic traits including cognitive ones, illustrating gene–culture coevolution after agriculture.)
  9. Adam Rutherford, Twitter post, 18 Oct 2022. (Science communicator Adam Rutherford explains the concept of a recent universal ancestor — relevant to how quickly traits or cultural innovations could spread through interbreeding.)
  10. Jaynes, Julian. The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1976. (Classic work proposing a late emergence of human introspective consciousness; serves as a conceptual springboard for theories like EToC, even if the specifics differ.)

  1. Pumpkinperson ↩︎ ↩︎ ↩︎ ↩︎

  2. Vectorsofmind ↩︎ ↩︎

  3. Snakecult ↩︎ ↩︎ ↩︎ ↩︎

  4. Snakecult ↩︎ ↩︎

  5. Snakecult ↩︎ ↩︎ ↩︎

  6. Aporiamagazine ↩︎ ↩︎

  7. Zygonjournal ↩︎

  8. Zygonjournal ↩︎

  9. Zygonjournal ↩︎ ↩︎ ↩︎

  10. Vectorsofmind ↩︎ ↩︎

  11. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  12. Pumpkinperson ↩︎ ↩︎

  13. Pumpkinperson ↩︎

  14. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  15. Zygonjournal ↩︎ ↩︎

  16. Zygonjournal ↩︎ ↩︎

  17. Zygonjournal ↩︎ ↩︎

  18. Zygonjournal ↩︎

  19. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  20. Vectorsofmind ↩︎ ↩︎

  21. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  22. Snakecult ↩︎

  23. Vectorsofmind ↩︎ ↩︎ ↩︎

  24. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  25. Vectorsofmind ↩︎ ↩︎

  26. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  27. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  28. Vectorsofmind ↩︎ ↩︎

  29. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  30. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  31. Julian Jaynes’s controversial bicameral mind theory (1976) argued that as recently as ~3,000 years ago humans were not self-aware in the modern sense; instead, they experienced hallucinated voices (interpreted as gods) directing their actions. While few scholars accept Jaynes’s late date, his idea that consciousness has a definable origin and wasn’t always with us inspires explorations like EToC 55 56↩︎

  32. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  33. Vectorsofmind ↩︎ ↩︎

  34. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  35. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  36. Snakecult ↩︎

  37. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  38. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  39. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  40. Snakecult ↩︎

  41. Snakecult ↩︎

  42. Snakecult ↩︎

  43. Snakecult ↩︎

  44. Pumpkinperson ↩︎

  45. Aporiamagazine ↩︎ ↩︎

  46. Aporiamagazine ↩︎

  47. Snakecult ↩︎ ↩︎

  48. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  49. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  50. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  51. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  52. Zygonjournal ↩︎

  53. Aporiamagazine ↩︎

  54. Zygonjournal ↩︎

  55. Vectorsofmind ↩︎

  56. Vectorsofmind ↩︎